We're sunsetting PodQuest on 2025-07-28. Thank you for your support!
Export Podcast Subscriptions
cover of episode Unveiling Olo — A Color Out of Oz!

Unveiling Olo — A Color Out of Oz!

2025/6/6
logo of podcast Short Wave

Short Wave

AI Deep Dive AI Chapters Transcript
People
A
Austin Rurda
Topics
Austin Rurda: 作为一名视光学和视觉科学教授,我很荣幸成为世界上第一个看到一种全新颜色的科学家,这种颜色名为Olo,它并非自然界固有,而是在实验室中通过精确的计算和激光技术创造出来的。Olo是一种比自然界中任何青色都更加饱和的蓝绿色,为了创造这种超饱和的颜色,我们团队采用了一种名为Oz的技术,灵感来源于电影《绿野仙踪》,旨在唤起一种全新的色彩感觉。我深知,每个人体验颜色的方式都有所不同,而色盲患者则因为基因的缺失,所看到的世界也与常人不同。为了创造Olo,我们必须绘制出视锥细胞的精确图谱,并精确刺激M视锥细胞,最终,我们成功地创造出了Olo,并用二进制代码010为其命名。虽然无法用颜料复制Olo的饱和度,但艺术家们通过其他方式,如添加荧光成分,来唤起人们对Olo的感受。虽然动物的视觉系统与人类大相径庭,它们所感知的颜色也与我们不同,但我相信,通过对Olo的研究,能够加深我们对颜色视觉的理解,并为未来的视觉科学研究开辟新的道路。为了让大家体验到Olo的感觉,我建议大家可以先看一段时间的红光,然后再看绿光,这样可以近似地感受到Olo的色彩。

Deep Dive

Chapters
This chapter explores the nature of color perception, explaining that it's a complex interplay of light, our three types of cone photoreceptors, and brain processing. It also highlights that individual experiences of color can vary due to differences in cone types and the possibility of color blindness.
  • Color is a complex process involving light, cone photoreceptors, and brain interpretation.
  • Humans have three cone types (L, M, S) sensitive to different light wavelengths.
  • Brain processes subtle differences in cone excitation to generate color perception.
  • Colorblindness results from missing one or more cone types.
  • Individual color perception varies due to unique cone arrangements.

Shownotes Transcript

Translations:
中文

Congress is considering a rescissions package from the White House that would claw back more than $1 billion of public media funding. Federal funding for all of public media amounts to about $1.60 per person per year. That helps bring you the news and podcasts you rely on from NPR. Please take a stand for public media today at GoACPR.org. You're listening to Shortwave from NPR.

Color is a trick of the light and a creation of our brain. It's actually very challenging to study color because of the complexity of it and how the perception is so context dependent. Austin Rurda is a professor of optometry and vision science at UC Berkeley, and he's likely the first person in the world to ever see a new color, meaning a color that does not exist in nature and was developed entirely in a lab.

Austin and his collaborator, computer scientist Ren Ong, call this novel color Olo. It's blue-green, it's a teal color, but it's just more saturated than any teal you can see in the natural world. And to make this super saturated color, the team used a technique they call Oz, named after the movie The Wizard of Oz, which, as you may remember, starts in black and white, until Dorothy emerges into a technicolor world.

Toto, I have a feeling we're not in Kansas anymore. We must be over the rainbow.

And so Oz, in a way, to me, it's that effort to evoke a new sensation of color. And so we go from a normal colored world to this extraordinarily colored world through direct manipulation of the self. Was it like that for you? Like, I see the world in basic color and this is super saturated? Well, I would say yes. But, you know, we're not looking at this in an IMAX theater. Yeah.

Our display is the size of an icon on your cell phone. Or it's the size of your fingernail held at arm's length. Today on the show, seeing Olo. How color perception works and how a swatch of color created by machines is pushing the boundaries of vision science. I'm Emily Kwong and you're listening to Shortwave from NPR.

This message comes from the Nature Conservancy. People from all walks of life depend on nature for the food they eat, the water they drink, and the air they breathe, for strengthening their communities, powering their livelihoods, and safeguarding their health. Nature is common ground for everyone, and uniting to protect nature can help solve today's challenges and create a thriving tomorrow for future generations. Discover why at nature.org slash NPR.

Public media is facing the most serious threat in its history. Congress is considering a White House proposal that would eliminate federal funding for the Corporation for Public Broadcasting, which helps fund local NPR stations. This move would immediately threaten many stations' ability to serve their communities and could force some to close. Take a stand for public media today at GoACPR.org. Okay, Austin and Ren, let's talk about color.

What is color, Austin? And for those of us who can see color, how are we able to? So humans have three types of cone photoreceptor, and they're sensitive to the long, middle, and short wavelengths of the visible light spectrum. So they're called L, M, and S. And with these three types of sensor, we can send information to the brain that will inform the brain about color.

So it's the brain that looks at the subtle differences in the excitation of those three cone types to generate a percept of the color. So with just a mere three cone types, humans are able to differentiate arguably up to 10 million different hues in the visual world. And that's really through the extensive processing that the brain does. It's a very important part of the process.

The three types of color cells in the retina, the reason they're sensitive to these three types of different parts of the visible spectrum is because they're filled with photopigments, which are proteins. And those proteins come from our DNA, from three genes. So literally, our color vision is baked into our DNA, literally.

Are different people seeing different colors? If all three of us here were to look at a sunflower, would the yellow of the petals look a little different because are our cones unique to us in some way?

Absolutely. And that people see colors in the world and experience them differently, for sure. And if you're a colorblind person, that is what we call dichromatic or hard dichromatic, is missing one of those three genes completely. And when that happens, then what is that vision like? Actually, it's really hard to know what the experience of having

another person's vision is. It's sort of impossible, right? And there's three types of this type of colorblindness. But the most common type would be an experience we think that sees the world only in shades of blue and yellow. Okay, so you don't see all the colors of the rainbow. You can't order the colors of the rainbow because you don't perceive them. Okay, you perceive them as shades of blue and yellow.

So absolutely, we're all seeing the world in different, you know, differently. Fascinating. Okay. Let's talk about your study. You set out to stimulate M cones without stimulating any neighboring L or S cones. And that doesn't happen in nature. So what did you want to know? If you did that, I guess the question becomes, do you see a square of color or not?

or is your brain just confused about what you see there? Do you see a black hole or, you know, what is it? And I thought, well, you know, I guess you would see a color and I wanted to know, Hey, does that look like a, what does it look like? Does it look like the greenest green you've ever seen? And I want to call out James Fong and Hannah Doyle amongst the many collaborators, but really they stand out as the people that did the hard work, had the perseverance and the smarts, the talent to really chase this down. It was so challenging. Uh,

But the fruits of the labor is so valuable because it is really something that's never been done before. So there's no charted course to it. Right. And you did see something. You saw Olo. What's required to see this novel color? Yeah, there are a number of parts. First of all, in order to be able to consider even targeting only the M cones, you have to have a map of the cone mosaic of the three types of cones. So...

Every subject in the study had to travel to the University of Washington to our collaborator's lab, where he has a device to image the retina. And with a special type of imaging called optical coherence tomography, he was able to label the cone types as being L, M, or S.

So he kind of mapped everyone's icons. That's right. So then when we go to the lab, in the lab here, there's a few steps. One is you need to dilate your pupil. And then we bite into a bite plate called a bite bar, which gets locked into the device.

So your head is held perfectly rigid, and then somebody else will align you in X, Y, and Z to get your pupil aligned with the output aperture of the system. This really reminds me of going to the eye doctor to test my vision. Go on. That's right. And then now there's one little...

important fact is that classifying the cones or generating maps of the cones is difficult. And nobody has ever mapped the cones right along the line of sight in an area called your fovea because the cones are really densely packed there. So we, instead, we mapped the cones a little bit away from the fovea, about a half a millimeter away from the center of the fovea. And initially, while everything's getting set up,

things look green or just a regular green, then once everything gets into place and you're carefully fixating, then you have this moment where it just turns this saturated teal. And I was aware at that moment that we had succeeded, that we had created and that we were able to stimulate only the M-cones.

And James Fong, the lead author on the paper, he invented the name OLO because it's a binary code for 010, which represent the stimulation of the L, M, and S cones. That's so smart. And as I understand it, you also had this teal laser next to OLO. And it was a laser containing the most saturated natural light you were able to generate. So study participants could compare the colors, right? That's kind of how you show that OLO isn't just teal, right?

It's a completely different color. That's right. What do you say to folks who wonder if Olo exists in nature in such a way that maybe other animals could see it? I love that question.

To be clear, animals don't see the world in color anything like a human does, right? Nothing like a human does. Animal eyes are vastly different than ours and even our closest cousins on the evolutionary tree, their genes for those photopigments that we talked about earlier,

They're not the same as for us. They don't have the same number as us, and they don't have the same genetic sequence. So its functional effect in the world for detecting light is totally different. We know that. Like hummingbirds, people have probably heard or may have heard, some species can see in UV light. We're blind to UV light as one example, but every animal sees it completely differently.

Another way you think about it is that, you know, we all look at a TV. We're like, wow, that color is pretty good. When your dog or your cat is sitting there looking at that TV, they do not see that and be like, wow, that kind of looks like, you know, that photo that we all took together outside the house this morning. It just, the colors don't look right. Okay.

How do you know? My cat can play the same video games as me. I think he's following. He might be following, but the colors won't look the same. Yeah. So back to this question about, you know, could there be, you know, an animal that could see Olo? I received that question first. Oh, what a great question. But it's actually that there's no way for that to happen because the experiential nature of the color for different species is just so vastly different. Yeah.

I recognize that you need a machine to see Olo, but for those of us at home, is there any way to approximate the Olo experience and to trick your eyes? Now, there's one type of situation where you can get an impression of what Olo might be, and that is if you...

desensitize, or if you're exposed to a bright red light for a period of time... I'm going to do this at home. I want to see Olo so bad. Okay. So if you look at red light and you kind of adapt to red light, by looking at red light too much for a long period of time, you may desensitize to it or adapt to it. And then immediately following the adaptation to red light, you show a green light, and that approximates...

the condition that we generate with OLO, whereby the M-cones are preferentially stimulated more than what normal natural light would do. And so if you wanted to get a rough idea of what OLO looked like, you could do this adaptation trick. Now, the difference is that when we deliver OLO, we can make it last. It can persist. You speak like such a steward of a color.

And it's so, it's funny to me because, like, the pop culture craze around seeing Olo has got to be pretty funny for you all to witness. I read about an artist from the UK, Stuart Semple, who is selling for $10,000 pre-orders of a paint based on Olo called YOLO. Yep. Which is, you know, you only live once. Oh, I know. We love that. What has been the funniest...

Olo homage or Olo plea that you've read about or seen? Well, I think I loved YOLO.

The reason I loved it, of course, you can't make a paint that recreates Olo. No. It seems like saturation is such a big part of this. That's right. It's all about saturation. It's not the hue. That's right. Stuart Semple totally realized that, and his paint was meant to sort of evoke a sensation, a feeling of Olo. Yeah. And from what he describes, he kind of achieved that by adding some fluorescent components. Some say, well, Olo's no different than Taco Bell's Baja Blast. Ha ha!

Okay. Some people say I had to color Olo on my Nike sneakers back in 2015. And so it's all been fun. They're experiencing FOMO Olo. That's what that is. FOMO Olo. Yes. We've had a bit of that. Fear of missing out on Olo. That's right.

Thank you for sharing Olo with us on Shortwave and with the world. And we wish you luck with future adventures and advances in human color vision science. Well, it's been a real pleasure to talk to you. Thanks for having us on, Emily.

Short Wavers, please like, follow, or subscribe to our show now. You will get a fun and fresh science episode in your feed four times a week. Today's episode was produced by Rachel Carlson. It was edited by Rebecca Ramirez. Tyler Jones checked the facts. Kweisi Lee was the audio engineer. Beth Donovan is our senior director, and Colin Campbell is our senior vice president of podcasting strategy. I'm Emily Kwong. Thanks for listening to Short Wave from NPR.

Thank you.

Give politics a chance with the NPR Politics Podcast, available wherever you get your podcasts. Hey everybody, it's Ian from How To Do Everything. On our show, we attempt to answer your how-to questions. We don't know how to do anything, so we call experts. Last season, both

Tom Hanks and Martha Stewart stop by to help. Our next season is launching in just a few months, so get us your questions now by emailing howto at npr.org or calling 1-800-424-2935.