We're sunsetting PodQuest on 2025-07-28. Thank you for your support!
Export Podcast Subscriptions
cover of episode The Trouble With Zero

The Trouble With Zero

2025/1/1
logo of podcast Short Wave

Short Wave

AI Deep Dive AI Insights AI Chapters Transcript
People
R
Regina Barber
Y
Yasmin Saplakoglu
Topics
Regina Barber: 本节目探讨了零的概念及其在人类大脑中的处理方式。零作为一种相对较新的概念,在历史上经历了从占位符到独立数字的演变过程,并曾引发过争议和误解。在神经科学方面,研究表明大脑中存在专门处理特定数字的神经元,并且处理小于4的数字和大于4的数字的方式不同。对零的研究发现,大脑将零置于数字线起始位置,但零在神经元活动方面仍表现出特殊性,这可能与大脑理解“缺失”的概念有关。 Yasmin Saplakoglu: 零的概念在历史上出现较晚,最初并非作为数字使用,而是一种占位符。在中世纪欧洲,零的概念曾引发恐惧和困惑,甚至被一些城市禁止。从神经科学的角度来看,大脑处理零的方式与处理其他数字的方式存在细微差别。一部分研究表明大脑处理零的方式与处理其他数字相似,零位于大脑中的数字线起始位置;另一部分研究则发现,更多神经元将零作为其偏好数字,这表明大脑可能用更多神经元来表示空集。这些研究结果的差异可能源于研究规模的不同,未来的研究将进一步探究大脑如何理解“缺失”以及如何处理更复杂的数字。 Yasmin Saplakoglu: 人脑中存在专门处理特定数字的神经元,这些神经元对特定数字的反应更强烈。大脑处理小于4的数字的方式与处理大于4的数字的方式不同,存在一个临界点4。当数字大于4时,大脑进行的是比较而非计数;当数字小于4时,大脑进行的是直接计数。大脑处理数字的方式可能与工作记忆或意识有关,因为人们只能记住有限数量的物体。研究人员试图探究大脑是否以特殊的方式处理零,并发现大脑将零置于数字线起始位置,但零在神经元活动方面仍表现出特殊性。未来的研究方向包括探究大脑如何理解“缺失”以及如何处理更复杂的数字。

Deep Dive

Key Insights

When was zero first used as a number, and by whom?

Zero was first used as a number around the 7th century by Indian mathematicians. They were the first to recognize zero as a digit, similar to other numbers like 1, 2, and 3.

Why was zero initially met with resistance in medieval Europe?

In medieval Europe, zero was met with resistance because it was seen as a confusing and even frightening concept. Philosophers and theologians associated it with chaos and disorder, and some even called it the 'devil's number.' Florence, Italy, went as far as banning zero altogether.

How does the brain process zero differently from other numbers?

Research shows that while the brain places zero at the start of a mental number line, similar to other numbers, it also processes zero differently in some ways. For instance, more neurons are activated for the concept of 'zero objects' compared to other small quantities, suggesting the brain represents absence with more neural activity.

What is the numerical distance effect, and how does it relate to zero?

The numerical distance effect is a phenomenon where the brain more easily distinguishes numbers that are far apart (e.g., 7 and 10) than those close together (e.g., 7 and 8). Researchers found that zero also exhibits this effect, suggesting the brain processes it similarly to other numbers, though with some unique characteristics.

How does the brain process small numbers differently from large numbers?

The brain processes small numbers (below four) with greater precision than larger numbers. For numbers above four, the brain shifts from counting to comparing, and this boundary is thought to be related to working memory, as people can typically hold about four objects in their awareness at once.

What are 'number neurons,' and how do they function in the brain?

Number neurons are specialized neurons in the brain that are tuned to specific numbers. For example, some neurons favor the number five and fire more when five items are present, while others favor different numbers. This system allows the brain to process numerical information in a highly specific way.

What future research directions are scientists exploring regarding zero?

Scientists are interested in further exploring how the brain comprehends zero and absence. One group aims to study the evolutionary steps toward understanding zero, while another is focusing on how the brain processes other abstract numbers, including the written word 'zero' compared to the digit or the concept of empty sets.

Chapters
The concept of zero is explored, tracing its historical development from a placeholder to a number. The show discusses the challenges people faced in accepting zero as a number and the abstract nature of zero compared to other numbers.
  • Zero was not initially a number but a placeholder.
  • Its adoption was met with resistance and even bans.
  • Zero's abstract nature makes it harder for the brain to process than other numbers.

Shownotes Transcript

Translations:
中文

This message comes from Mint Mobile. From the gas pump to the grocery store, inflation is everywhere. So Mint Mobile is offering premium wireless starting at just $15 a month. To get your new phone plan for just $15, go to mintmobile.com slash switch. You're listening to Shortwave from NPR. Hey, Shortwavers, it's Regina Barber. And today is the first day of 2025. Happy New Year! Happy New Year!

The new year is all about blank slates, new beginnings, starting from scratch. And so we thought, what better time than now to focus on the number that signifies origin points, literally starting from nothing, zero. So zero was invented relatively late in history. It was first thought to be invented around like 2,500 years ago by Babylonian traders in ancient Mesopotamia, actually. That's Yasmin Soplikoluuk.

She's a science writer at Quantum Magazine. Back then, they used a symbol like two slanted wedges on clay tablets. But at the time, it wasn't a number yet. It was really used as a placeholder so that you can distinguish between different types of numbers like 20 or 250 or 205. And Yasmin says that this idea of a placeholder wasn't totally unique. The ancient Maya, for example, had a little shell symbol that they used in a similar way.

But zero didn't really become a number on its own until around the 7th century. There were Indian mathematicians who came up with a couple of ways to use zero as a number. And they were the kind of first to figure out that zero could be a digit, just like the other numbers, like 1 and 2 and 3.

After that, it kind of went out from India to the Arab world. And then, you know, in the 13th century, Fominachi actually picked up the idea during his travels in North Africa. And he brought it back to medieval Europe, you know, along with the base 10 number system. But in medieval Europe...

Everyone was thrilled about this concept of zero. People had difficulty with accepting it. It was kind of scary. People were confused by it. Some thought of it as like the devil's number that challenged like really deeply held ideas. And, you know, because of the influence of the church, like philosophers and theologians associated nothing with like chaos and disorder. One city, Florence, Italy, actually banned the number zero altogether.

It's a weird concept if you even think too deeply about it. It's like we're describing something that doesn't exist, right? We like see things.

three chairs or we see four birds and we can count those and they're, you know, physically there. But we don't see zero birds or zero chairs. We just know that they're absent. But that is zero. That's, you know, an extra level of abstraction from the other kinds of numbers that we see around us all the time. And that abstraction actually makes it harder for our brains to process. ♪

So today on the show, the neuroscience of the number zero. How do humans think about the concept of nothing? How do we find out? And what does that mean for our brains? You're listening to Shortwave, the science podcast from NPR. This message comes from Charles Schwab. When it comes to managing your wealth, Schwab gives you more choices, like full-service wealth management and advice when you need it. You can also invest on your own and trade on Thinkorswim. Visit Schwab.com to learn more.

This message comes from Capella University. Learning doesn't have to get in the way of life. With Capella's game-changing FlexPath learning format, you can set your own deadlines and learn on your own schedule. That means you don't have to put your life on hold to earn your degree. Instead,

Enjoy learning your way and pursue your educational and career goals without missing a beat. A different future is closer than you think with Capella University. Learn more at capella.edu.

Support for NPR and the following message come from Bowlin Branch. Change your sleep with Bowlin Branch's airy blankets, cloud-like duvets, and breathable sheets. Feel the difference with 15% off your first order at bowlinbranch.com with code NPR. Exclusions apply. See site for details.

Okay, Yasmin, before we get into, like, the complexity of zero, let's just start with, like, the neuroscience of numbers in general. You've done some writing about this, like how our brains comprehend small numbers differently than large numbers. Can you tell us more about that?

Right. So this is actually kind of cool. We have what are called number neurons in the brain. So there are neurons that are tuned to specific numbers. There are neurons that favor, for example, the number five, and there are other neurons that favor the number seven.

And so on. So that means that for the neurons that favor the number five, they'll fire more. Like if it sees five items on a table, it'll fire more than if it sees four or...

you know, six. It'll still fire for four and six, but less. And then even less for, you know, three and seven. Yeah. So these neurons are specifically like for that number. They like really like that number. Yeah. Yeah. It's like kind of neat that we have all of these and there's a lot of questions surrounding it still. Like we don't know if there are neurons that fire for like 1,505. Yeah.

That seems like there would be a lot of neurons in the brain then for the various numbers. I'm sure there's some other kind of mechanism there. And yeah, there was this research recently that found that the brain actually analyzes small numbers different than it does larger numbers. So there's a weird boundary around the number four. Yeah.

There seems to be some sort of double mechanism that's happening. So the brain processes numbers that are smaller than four in a more precise way than it does

for numbers larger than four. Yeah, we did an episode about this way back at the beginning of last year and how if you get above four, you're not counting, you're actually comparing. And when you're below four, that's when you're literally like your brain is counting. Yeah. So you're not counting anymore past four. So interesting, right? It's like...

I think about this all the time where it's like, why is it four? Like, why not five? But it's four. And when I reported that piece, a couple of experts were talking to me about how it's also weirdly related to working memory or awareness. So people can only really hold a certain number of objects in their awareness. And that's four.

Four. So they think that maybe there's some sort of connection there between, you know, how we're processing numbers and working memory. Yeah, it's like phone numbers, right? We're in chunks of three and four to like,

Right. So fairly recently, these two researchers were like, hey, if zero is special in math and history, maybe, and this is what you were saying, maybe it's special in neuroscience. Maybe we think about it differently. Right.

One group looked at patients with epilepsy. They already had these like electrodes in their brain. So the researchers were able to see like how individual neurons were firing. Yeah. And the other one? The other group looked more at populations of neurons. So it was kind of a larger scale. They used a magnetoencephalography scanner. That sounds right. That was impressive. Long word.

And basically, that means that, you know, as the neurons fire, they generate voltages, which creates magnetic fields that the machine can detect. And by analyzing the magnetic fields, the researchers were able to kind of probe what the neurons were doing when the participants were prompted to think about zero. That's so cool. Yeah. So these researchers, they're looking at neurons either specifically or as a group, you know, firing. Right.

What did they find? Was there like a new discovery on how we think about zero?

So they actually, they were looking for something called the numerical distance effect, which is basically a phenomenon that occurs when the brain processes non-zero numbers. And it means that it can more easily distinguish numbers that are far apart from each other than those that are close together. So the brain has a little bit more difficulty distinguishing between like seven and eight versus seven and 10, for example, or seven and 11. Got it. Okay. So-

The idea for both these groups was that we should see if zero also is part of this numerical distance effect, because if it is, then the brain might be seeing zero just as it does the other numbers. Just a normal number. Just a normal number. Yeah. Yeah. Probably not. Kind of. What happened? Yeah. So the first group, the one that looked at a bigger scale, found that the brain was

processes zero similarly to other numbers. Basically, it puts zero at the start of a mental number line, like it's before one. It showed the numerical distance effect, which is what they were looking for. So their conclusion was there's no difference in the way that the brain sees zero than the other numbers. Zero in terms of both the digit zero and like zero objects. The second group

Also found that the brain puts zero before one on the mental number line, but they found subtle differences that still made zero special in the brain. So, for example, they found that more neurons had zero as their preferred number than other small numbers. That suggested to them that the brain might be representing, you know, this empty set with more neurons.

than it does for other small quantities. But this was only true for quantity zero. For the digit zero, they did not find any difference. Like the brain saw the digit zero like it does the other digits, like one, two, three. So as you're doing this reporting, do you find that these conclusions are complementary? Do you think that they're like...

fighting each other. What do you think? Yeah, so I actually, it was funny because I had both groups kind of look at each other's results and I was like, what's going on here? It's slightly different. And they both said that they think, you know, their results are complementary actually. And the reason for the discrepancy was most likely just scale. And, you know, they're hopeful that future experiments will be able to kind of tease apart, you know, what the nuances of this are.

So, like, what other, like, research did they and did the researchers talk about, like, that they would like to do? Yeah, I think that, I mean, there's a lot of directions you can go from here. Like, it's

how the brain comprehends zero, this was kind of the first step into that. And now, you know, one of the groups, actually, they're hoping to go more in the direction of understanding how the brain comprehends absence, because they think that if they can kind of compare how the brain is, uh,

processing zero and processing absence, they might be able to see how, like evolutionarily, how the steps toward understanding zero happen. And then the other group, they're more interested in the numbers aspect of things. So they are hoping to look more into some of these maybe stranger numbers. But for example, like

no one looked at the written word zero, which would be a really interesting thing to look at too. Like, would that look different in the brain than the digit zero or than it would for empty sets? Right. What did you like take away from this reporting? Like you're, you're working on this, you know, you're learning about zero and the history. Like what was your like big takeaway when you're talking to all, all of these researchers? I,

I think my big takeaway is how incredible and complicated and big the brain is. I just, I think my mind is still blown that we have neurons that are attuned to specific numbers and ways to comprehend these abstract things.

you know, ideas because numbers and zero especially is an abstraction and we somehow figured out a way to comprehend it, which is incredible to me. Yeah. Yasmin, thank you so much for bringing us the story on zero. Of course. Thank you for being interested. It was super fun to report. So I'm glad I'm glad others find it cool, too.

If you want to hear more about how numbers can be tricky for us to comprehend, that episode is called How Big Numbers Break Our Brains. We'll link to it in our show notes. This episode was produced by Hannah Chin and edited by our showrunner, Rebecca Ramirez. Tyler Jones checked the facts. Jimmy Keeley was the audio engineer. Beth Donovan is our senior director. And Colin Campbell is our senior vice president of podcasting strategy. I'm Regina Barber. Thank you for listening to Shorewave from NPR.

This message comes from Mint Mobile. From the gas pump to the grocery store, inflation is everywhere. So Mint Mobile is offering premium wireless starting at just $15 a month. To get your new phone plan for just $15, go to mintmobile.com slash switch.

For every headline, there's also another story about the people living those headlines. On weekdays, Up First brings you the day's biggest news. On Sundays, we bring you closer with a single story about the people, places, and moments reshaping our world. Your news made personal. Every Sunday on the Up First podcast from NPR.

Every January, millions of people take the pledge to cut down on alcohol in the new year. If you're one of them, count on Life Kit, NPR's self-help podcast, for tips and tricks you can use to make the most out of your commitment. We'll help you draw up plans and have experts weigh in on how to stay motivated and kind to yourself throughout the month. Search Life Kit's Dry January wherever you get your podcasts for the tools you need to pull it off. From NPR.