We're sunsetting PodQuest on 2025-07-28. Thank you for your support!
Export Podcast Subscriptions
cover of episode Flight simulator for moths reveals they navigate by starlight

Flight simulator for moths reveals they navigate by starlight

2025/6/18
logo of podcast Nature Podcast

Nature Podcast

AI Deep Dive AI Chapters Transcript
People
B
Benjamin Thompson
D
Dan Fox
E
Elena Sherry
E
Eric Warrant
N
Nick Pachuchal
Topics
Benjamin Thompson: 本期节目主要讨论了飞蛾如何利用星光导航以及人类如何在7万年前扩展到新的生态系统中。飞蛾利用星光导航是一个引人入胜的话题,我们将深入探讨飞蛾如何利用星光进行长途迁徙,以及它们如何适应不同的环境条件。这项研究不仅揭示了飞蛾的导航机制,也为我们理解动物行为和生态适应提供了新的视角。 Nick Pachuchal: 很少有动物像人类和一些候鸟那样使用星星导航,而昆虫中只有蜣螂被认为使用星星导航,但距离很短。每年有大量的飞蛾迁徙到它们从未去过的特定地点,然后返回,因此能够找到方向对它们非常重要。新的研究表明,飞蛾除了利用地球磁场导航外,还能利用银河系。我将采访研究作者Eric Warrant,以了解更多关于这项研究的细节。 Eric Warrant: 我研究的飞蛾外表普通,但具有非凡的自然历史,是一种长途夜间迁徙者,能够从繁殖地找到从未去过的阿尔卑斯山洞穴。这些飞蛾从澳大利亚东南部飞行约1000公里到达澳大利亚最高的山脉中的高山洞穴,那里聚集了约40亿只飞蛾。飞蛾迁徙可能是为了逃避繁殖地的酷热,因为它们不耐热。我们发现飞蛾有两个指南针:恒星指南针和磁力指南针,它们同时使用这两个指南针,如果其中一个消失,它们可以默认使用另一个。飞蛾的眼睛很小,限制了它们能看到的星星数量,但飞蛾的眼睛结构使它们能更鲜明地看到周围的世界,它们很可能比我们更生动、更详细地看到银河。

Deep Dive

Chapters
Bogong moths undertake an extraordinary 1000km migration across Australia. This study reveals their navigational strategies, using a combination of celestial cues and the Earth's magnetic field.
  • Bogong moths migrate 1000km across Australia using the Milky Way and Earth's magnetic field.
  • They possess two compasses: a stellar compass and a magnetic compass.
  • The moths' small eyes allow them to see the Milky Way's brightness gradient for navigation.

Shownotes Transcript

Translations:
中文

Heather is a nurse practitioner from UnitedHealthcare. We meet patients wherever they live. During a house call, she found Jack had an issue. Jack's blood pressure was dangerously high. It was 217 over 110. So they got Jack to the hospital and got him the help he needed. He had had a stent placed in his heart, preventing a massive heart attack.

If it wasn't for my guardian angel, I wouldn't be here. Hear more stories like Jack's at UnitedHealthcare.com. Benefits, features, and or devices vary by plan, area, limitation, and exclusions apply. With the Redfin app, you'll know the moment your next place hits the market. Whether you're looking to buy your dream home or rent a sweet apartment, give Redfin your gotta-have-it wish list of property features today.

And you'll receive real-time notifications tailored just for you. Ready to see it up close and personal? Scheduling a tour is just a tap away. Don't wait to find your perfect match. Download the Redfin app and start searching today. Nature.

Welcome back to The Nature Podcast. This week, the moths that are guided by the stars. And how human populations expanded into new ecosystems 70,000 years ago. I'm Benjamin Thompson. And I'm Nick Pachuchal. Bogong moths can use the stars to navigate, according to a new study in Nature.

Few animals are known to use the stars to find their way. Humans, famously, have used them throughout history, and some migratory birds are thought to be able to. Other than that, the list isn't very long, as far as we know. When it comes to insects, only dung beetles are thought to use the stars to navigate, but even then, only over short distances. Bogong moths, though, are insects that travel great distances.

Each year, an enormous number of these moths migrate across Australia to a very specific location that they've never been to, and later they make the return journey. Clearly, being able to find their way is of great importance to these insects.

Evidence suggests that bogong moths use the Earth's magnetic field to help them find their way. But now the new study shows that they have another tool in their navigatory arsenal, the Milky Way. I caught up with study author Eric Warrant to find my way through the study.

I started by asking him to tell me a bit more about these curious moths. They're a very nondescript little moth. It doesn't look much different than any other brown moth most people are kind of aware of, but it has a quite remarkable natural history compared to most moths. This moth is a long-distance migrant at night, and it's able to find...

its way from its breeding areas in spring, about 1,000 kilometres to a place it's never been before, which are an assembly of alpine caves in Australia's highest mountains in the extreme southeast of the country. And these moths have come from all over southeastern Australia, about 4 billion of them. And that's obviously a really long trip. Why is it that these moths are doing that? We don't know exactly, but it's very likely that they're attempting to escape Australia

becoming heat in the breeding areas where they've departed. And these areas are in semi-arid areas of southeastern Australia. They can get very, very hot in summer. And we do know from preliminary measurements that they don't tolerate heat so well. So at some point during their evolution, they've

develop this strategy of leaving these areas and going to the coolest place that exists in Australia in summer, which are high alpine areas. And so, as you said, this is a 1,000 kilometre journey that they've never done before. So I guess the question at the centre of your paper is how do they find their way? What have been the ideas behind

before your study on how exactly they might have done this? Well, I must admit we got our clues about how they might be doing this from nocturnal birds that migrate long distances. For instance, European birds that travel from Europe to Africa every year. We know already that they use the Earth's magnetic field and they also use the stars to

And we had already found about eight years ago that the bogong moth has a magnetic sense from in a separate set of experiments. And so we started to think, well, maybe they're able to use the Earth's magnetic field. But we also in those earlier experiments realized that the magnetic sense was used together with visual landmark information.

And we were very curious, really, about what kind of visual information these moths could be using. And the stars became an obvious thing to investigate. And so how do you go about determining whether they're using the stars to navigate? Do you just watch them and see if they look at the stars every now and again? Or how is it done? Well, the way we did this experiment actually was in very controlled conditions.

We built a lab in Australia where we did these experiments and we captured the moths during their migration in the spring on their way to the mountains and in the autumn on their way from the mountains just by using a lamp with a big sheet in a tree in the wilderness.

and we transferred the moths that we caught to a so-called flight simulator, which is just a kind of a cylinder that sits on the table. And we suspended the moth in the middle of this cylinder on a long thin axle that allowed the moth, once attached to this axle, was able to fly vigorously in any direction it wished. And then above the moth, we actually projected an image of the starry night sky using a projector, a highly realistic image

of the sky outside the lab on the day and time of the experiment. And in order to make sure that the moth was not using the Earth's magnetic field as its cue for navigation, we had the whole arena surrounded in a set of three-dimensional Helmholtz coils

which allowed us to completely control the Earth's magnetic field and in this particular experiment actually get rid of the Earth's magnetic field entirely. So there was in fact no field present around the moth. And then we just simply watched what the moth did under the natural sky. Which way did it fly? Was it confused or could it fly in its appropriate migratory direction? And we found that

That moth after moth after moth flew in its appropriate migratory direction. And if we rotated the starry night sky 180 degrees, they turned and flew in exactly the opposite direction. And if we took all the stars and randomly distributed them across this projection screen, then the moths were totally disoriented. And that was what made us realize that they could actually fly.

see the stars and use them as a compass to find a micro tree direction relative to north. I wonder though, the night sky changes quite a lot. The stars aren't always visible. It could be cloudy as the earth moves, the stars move relative to it. So,

how is this a good cue for them to, you know, navigate and find their way? That would be a bad idea to only have that single compass because, as you say, if the stars are completely covered by cloud, then that information is gone and they would be disoriented. But,

We do know now that when we actually had the moths in our flight arena, but this time not in the lab, but up on the hill behind the lab under the natural Australian sky, then we discovered that they were able to orient under the natural stars in the right direction as expected.

But even on nights where the stars were completely covered by cloud, they could still do it, which implied that they were using something else as a compass. And because we already knew that they had the ability to detect the Earth's magnetic field, we deduced that it must have been the Earth's magnetic field that they fell back on when the stars were covered by clouds. So what we effectively found was that the moths had

have two compasses, a stellar compass and a magnetic compass, and that they use both. And if one disappears for whatever reason, because of cloud, for instance, covering the sky, then they can default to the other, in this case, geomagnetic compass. And no offence to any moths listening, but they have quite tiny brains and they

I wouldn't have thought the best eyes to be able to see the stars. So did you figure out how they were actually using their cues despite these limitations? Yes, that's a very good question because the eyes, as you say, are very, very small and this limits how many stars they can actually see because the number of stars in the night time sky that you can actually see is determined by the size of your pupil in the dark. And in the dark, our pupil is about 8mm across and

and it's about a tenth that in a bogong moth at night. So the number of actual stars they can see is extremely low. But in comparison to our eyes, the optics of the bogong moth eye are built in such a way that

the extended world, that is to say the world all around us that we normally look at, is much much brighter for them than it is for us if we look at the same scene. And because the Milky Way is effectively an extended object in the sky, this means that the moths are very likely able to see the Milky Way much more vividly and brightly.

and in much greater detail than we can. And we think that this may be key to the compass actually, because if you see the Milky Way in the southern night sky, this is very different to the northern hemisphere, but in the southern hemisphere, the Milky Way is extremely prominent

and it varies in its intensity, actually, so that it becomes brighter and brighter the more southwards you look into the Milky Way. And this gradient of intensity may be part of the mechanism by which the bogong moth is able to use the night sky as a compass, but that's still a speculation. We haven't investigated that yet in detail. And do you think this is just something that these moths have, or what you found here, do you think it

could be found in other insects or other animals? Absolutely. We think this, of course, and we already know, for instance, that birds very likely use a very similar compass mechanism to navigate between Europe and Africa. But when it comes to other insects, there's no reason why this wouldn't be a useful thing for other nocturnal moths

that migrate long distances to use, even though the vast majority of moths that migrate do so from one broad latitudinal region to another, typically between north and south, one direction or the other. But nonetheless, to find that southward or northward direction, it would be very useful to employ compasses just like the bogong moth. That was Erik Warrant from the University of Lund in Sweden. For more on that story, check out the show notes for a link to the paper. Coming up.

evidence that humans moved to live in many different ecosystems across Africa, shortly before migrating out of the continent. Right now though, it's time for the research highlights with Dan Fox. Nigeria's pangolins are under threat, mainly because they're considered delicious. Pangolins are among the most trafficked animals in the world, in part because their scales are used for traditional medicine in Asia.

Nigeria has become a global hub for pangolin trafficking, but the reasons why people are going after these animals has been unclear. To better understand why hunters poach pangolins in Nigeria, researchers interviewed 809 hunters and meat vendors in the country's southeastern region.

They found that scales were not the main driver. Instead, 98% of the pangolin's court were used for meat. The animals were either eaten by the hunters or sold at a local market.

The survey respondents also gave pangolin meat the highest palatability score of all listed meats. They suggest that conservationists should focus more on shifting local behaviours through education and food safety programmes rather than concentrate on cracking down on scale trafficking. Find that research in Nature, Ecology and Evolution. The galaxy Andromeda and our Milky Way have long been thought to be on a collision course.

But some researchers have now cast doubt on this intergalactic merger taking place. The Milky Way and Andromeda are the two largest galaxies in the cosmic neighbourhood called the Local Group, meaning that their gravitational pull dominates the region. But roughly 100 smaller galaxies in the area also exert their own gravitational pull.

Researchers modelled the dynamics of the local group, including the third and fourth largest members, and found that one of their trajectories may pull the Milky Way sideways as Andromeda approaches, giving a 50% chance that the collision will be avoided for at least the next 10 billion years. Collide with that research in Nature Astronomy.

Next up on the show, new research suggests that Homo sapiens massively expanded the range of ecosystems they lived in not long before our species made its first successful migration outside of the African continent. Genetic studies indicate that contemporary Eurasian individuals can trace a huge amount of their ancestry back to a small group of Homo sapiens that migrated out of Africa around 50,000 years ago.

But this wasn't the first time our species attempted this journey. There's lots of evidence of human activity outside of Africa long before this, but none of these populations appear to have gone the distance. A big question is, why? There are many competing ideas, but to help shed light on this question, a group of researchers have a paper in Nature in which they've combined data from climate models with information gained from archaeological sites across Africa.

Their findings suggest that in the lead-up to the successful migration, humans expanded their habitat range across Africa, a move which may have helped set them up for their later travels. To find out more about the work, I spoke to one of the team, Elena Sherry, from the Max Planck Institute of Geoanthropology in Germany. I asked her to lay out some of the other ideas as to why the migration 50,000 years ago may have been successful.

Some of the hypotheses that have been proposed are very difficult to prove. So things like, oh, maybe grandparents were living longer and this affected, you know, survivability of children and there was a population increase. Or, you know, other ideas that have been proposed, there was some kind of technological breakthrough, but no one's ever been able to find any evidence from the archaeological record that looks profoundly different beyond Africa as opposed to inside Africa. And some studies also spoke about potential consequences

populations within Africa before 50,000 years ago, which was sort of interesting. The data from the genetic record potentially reflects this. Yes, one of the reasons that some folk think led up to humans migrating out of Africa 50,000 years ago is that they moved to live in a bunch of different places and different ecosystems and

And perhaps in some sense, this provided the skills or the knowledge, obviously hard to know, that ultimately allowed for successful migration out of the continent. And this is central to your work. You're looking at the expansion of modern humans, Homo sapiens, into new ecosystems, new niches within the continent of Africa, right? Yes. And this ties up with some of the work that came out of the genetic studies that spoke of Homo sapiens.

Population movements is exactly the same time. And to look for more evidence that this expansion preceded outward migration, you've combined archaeological work with climate work, looking specifically at the time 120,000 years ago to 14,000 years ago. Why have you done this? Basically, to be able to understand habitats and niches, we need to have a rounded picture from...

climatic variables. We need to know about vegetation, the bios that were present because these also shift through time, right? So for the last 120,000 years, a patch of rainforest wasn't always a patch of rainforest. There are shifts because there were changes in rainfall, there's changes in temperature. So you have this dynamic shifting environment. And with enough, you have humans moving around. For the archaeological data, what we have is we know where archaeological sites were and we know how old they are. So we can route those sites and

So you've got this situation then where you've got physical evidence of human habitation and you've matched that up with your modelling of what the climate may have looked like in different periods.

By combining these, what do your results show about how humans might have moved around before they migrated out of the continent? What we see from that work is that there is an overall niche space. And by that, I mean there's an overall space in Africa suitable for human habitation. And that's going to exclude extremely arid deserts, you know, places like that. That background niche space and the

the human exploitation of various niches in Africa track each other pretty well until 70,000 years ago where we see this decoupling. We see the overall niche space decreasing, but we see human niche space expanding. So for example, from the studies, it's obvious that people like ecotonal environments. So these are border areas between very different ecosystems. So you might have a grassland and then you have a forest and the area in between them is sort of this ecotonal mixed environments. And

And, you know, people did exploit those environments in the past because you can get mixed resources. But it looks very much like overall as a population in Africa, after about 70,000 years ago, we see a much more intensive engagement with different ecosystems. That speaks to a need and an ability to be able to do so that we just don't see in the record before. So what you're saying is that before 70,000 years,

years ago humans were moving around areas that were perhaps the most habitable between forests and savannah for example but as you get closer to this 70 000 year mark they suddenly expand where they live and live in lots of different environments yes and i sort of want to emphasize that it's not like before 70 000 years ago people were just exploiting grasslands they did exploit different ecosystems and we know they did we have archaeological evidence for it but the

intensity of exploitation changes around 70,000 years ago. And what does your evidence suggest may have caused this? Because you're looking at archaeological evidence and climate modelling evidence. Is there something in one or the other that suggests why this might have happened?

So if we look at the climate and we look at, for example, climate variables that represent preferred human habitats, so certain temperature, certain rainfall, etc., we can see that obviously this is very dynamic over time in Africa. Sometimes these spaces with these suitable variables increase and sometimes they decrease. And after 70,000 years ago, we see a decrease. So it is not good times for those human populations in Africa.

And this is somehow leading them to adapt to

And this form of adaptation means that they are moving and exploiting into environments that perhaps they had limited experience with before, exploited in a limited way before, but that they're fully engaging with these environments. And your evidence then suggests that Homo sapiens are expansively living in different places on the continent about 70,000 years ago, which obviously is not that long before humans are thought to have migrated out 50,000 years ago. Yes, that's right.

And the question there is, how has the former helped the latter? Is it possible to know? We think that it helped people to develop systems within their society, within their knowledge base, to cope with adapting to very different environments. We don't know exactly what this would have involved, so it could be

developments in how people keep in touch with one another, some sort of cultural, political invention that's not visible in the archaeological record. We don't really know why or what exactly it was, but we do know that this happens during a period of environmental downturn, that they start turning to exploit more habitats, probably out of need.

And that this pressure delivers an ability to be more flexible, to become specialists in different habitats over time. And that this...

speaks to our ability to then move beyond Africa. Obviously, though, a lot of this work is based on modelling and based on archaeological evidence. We know that in this field, a single find can kind of upend things and likewise changes in the climate modelling. There are folk who will read your paper and say, do you know what? This is evidence that this may have happened, but there are a dozen other things as well. What would you say to that? I mean, it's true that we have a lot of sparse data for these time periods, and that's particularly true of fossils.

which is why we decided to go for archaeological sites over the last 120,000 years where the climate variables are more robust. And of course, like my colleagues, they use methods to deal with bias. For example, the further you go back in time, the less sites you're going to find simply because of the attrition rate of preservation.

You know, we had to account for these things. And your paper's out now and you've put forward your evidence. What does this not answer? Because this is a debate that will run in the community for a while yet, I suppose. It doesn't tell us how they were doing these things or what specific mechanisms, biological, cultural, were in place that allowed people to respond in this way because, you know, there were other downturns in the past that didn't result in this behaviour. Perhaps...

between those time periods where population increases that create greater pressures. We can't answer that yet. There may be a time when we can if ancient DNA ever goes back far enough. But if it's a cultural story that we need archaeology for, it's going to be much harder to answer. And if it's something invisible, like a social adaptation, that's going to be very, very difficult to show in any form. But we can see that we know it happened.

And we know people left Africa and we are providing a part of the answer. That was Eleanor Sherry from the Max Planck Institute of Geoanthropology in Germany. For more on that story, check out the show notes for some links.

Finally on the show, it's time for the briefing chat, where we discuss a couple of articles that have been highlighted in the Nature Briefing. Ben, what have you been reading this week? Well, I've got a story about breathing, Nick. It's published in the journal Marine Mammal Science, and I read about it on Science Alert. And it's not about humans breathing, it's about humpback whales. And a study looking at

ways that they breathe and something that might be an effort to try and communicate with us. Well humpback whales are certainly getting a lot of love on the podcast recently a couple weeks ago you might remember I talked about how they see but this is about how they communicate maybe what have the researchers been looking into here? Yeah this all comes down to blowing bubbles okay now humpbacks

do that a lot, right? They use their blowholes and their mouths, and they do it for lots of different reasons. Males do it in breeding grounds. It's like an antagonistic display, for example. It's used to corral prey, all sorts of things, right? And there can be lots of different sorts of bubbles. These quite intricate spiral bubble nets they use to catch their food. But this work in particular centers on these kind of perfectly circular bubble rigs.

rings, these kind of donut shapes. And there's a picture of one of these in the article, and it's quite something. I mean, I'm imagining sort of like a smoke ring sort of thing, but with bubbles. Yeah, basically, that's it, right? Maybe a couple of metres across, and they break the surface, as bubbles tend to do, in this kind of perfect shape.

And a team, as I say, have been looking at these. And they've been looking at images and recordings of this, taken either below the water by swimmers or above the water from people on boats or planes or using drones, that sort of thing. And they've found something maybe unexpected. I mean, you mentioned at the start that this may be a way for them to communicate with us. So are these rings their attempt to be like, hey, humans, here we are? It's a big potential. This is one way that this research...

has been surmised, that it could be an attempt to communicate. So specifically, they looked at 12 ring-producing events, if I can describe it like that, which contained 39 of these rings in total, some of which were made by more than one whale, sort of like a group of whales. And so they've had a look at these and what's going on, and 10 of these events happened near a boat or a human swimmer, and six of these involved multiple whales. And in many cases, this is associated with whales' abductions,

approaching a boat or swimmers. And the way it's been described, they're showing quite playful behavior. It's not like they're showing any sort of aggression or antagonistic behavior. And what's interesting is in this work, the team have also included data from a separate multi-year study, which used drones, hundreds of flights. And this didn't observe any bubble rings at all. So based on this limited data set,

it seems like there's potentially an association between humans and these whales doing this particular activity. And if they are indeed trying to communicate with us, what are they trying to say? That's a great question, Nick. And I went back and rewatched Star Trek IV, The Voyage Home, several times to try and figure that out. Didn't help.

But I will say that it's not just humpback whales. It seems that lots of other whale species will approach boats and they will blow bubbles. Maybe not these perfect circles, but, you know, they do show bubble like behavior. But these haven't really been studied very much. But what's interesting is that bubbles have been interpreted as a way that whales communicate between themselves.

So double underline maybe, but it could be that in some way they're trying to get a message across, whatever that may be, between people and themselves. And I guess we'll watch this space or watch these bubbles, I guess, to see if researchers can uncover exactly what they're saying. But that's super interesting. And it's also related to my story this week.

because it's also about breathing. So I've been reading an article in Nature this week based on a study in Current Biology about how your breath can be used to identify you and potentially as a diagnostic tool. Right, so what, breaths are like fingerprints? Basically, yes. According to this study, each person's breath is unique to them.

So the understanding of breathing is that it's very closely related to the brain. The reason that you breathe is to give your brain and other organs oxygen and so therefore, you know, these things are very closely related and everyone's brain is unique. So these researchers thought, well, if everyone's

brain is unique, maybe their breathing is as well. And so they looked at the breathing of 97 healthy people for 24 hours, and then they trained a machine learning algorithm on the different aspects of the breathing that they could detect, and they showed that most of the time they could identify specifically who it was just based on their breathing. To me, that's quite unexpected. But of course,

I guess, is this just regular relaxed breathing? Because I'm someone who likes to do a lot of sport. Obviously, when I'm swimming, I'm breathing in a different way to when I'm talking to you, recording this podcast. So is there one specific sort of breath that each person has? Yeah, there's a specific sort of breath that different people have. So there is basically asymmetry between how you breathe through one nostril versus the other, how quickly you breathe.

all this sort of information can be used. And it's interesting that you mention activity as well, because the researchers were less well able to identify people while they were sleeping. So people's sleep breathing was more similar than when they were awake. But

But the way this research worked is they created a little device attached to the back of your neck and then went into your nostrils. And then people just went around their daily activities with this. And then they got all the data on how they're breathing. And then they were able to do it to, as I say, train this machine learning algorithm to identify the people and detect certain things about their breathing. Because the other thing they were interested in was could this be used as some sort of diagnostic tool?

So they gathered data on the participants' BMIs and also their levels of depression and anxiety. And this is a correlation, but they saw that there was a link between the way they breathed and these particular things. So this could be used to identify that someone has depression, for example. Right, because the baseline breathing, this kind of calm, solo breathing is one fingerprint, but then in a different situation, you can see something else. And that could be used to identify a situation that needs

needs attention. Yeah, that's the idea that they could basically see if someone has a condition that maybe their healthcare professional needs to be aware of or something like that. But also, you could go the other way as well. There is evidence to suggest that how you breathe and controlling your breathing and doing sort of breathing exercises can help with

with depression and anxiety. So they want to collect further data on the breathing profiles of people to see if there are ways to sort of make the breathing more healthy and therefore make the people themselves healthier. Right, because I covered a story a while back about in mice, if you massively accelerate their heart, you can induce anxious behaviours in

So people often think about the body and the brain as two separate things, but it seems like there's a body of evidence, if you will, that's coalescing around this idea that really they are part of this one holistic thing. Yeah, and researchers interviewed in this article have said that the most potent tool available to us is our breath. Like, you know, the US military...

teaches its personnel to control their breath, to manage stress and keep focus in high pressure moments. So the potential is there for this to be used and for more diagnostics to be done in the future. But yeah, more to come from this, I think. Well, fascinating story there, Nick. Thank you for bringing it to the briefing chat. And listeners, for more on those stories and where you can sign up to The Nature Briefing to get more like them delivered directly to your inbox, check out the show notes for some links.

And while you're there, you'll also find a link to a survey, or you can head to go.nature.com forward slash podsurvey2025. We're looking to find out a little bit more about all of you who listen to the podcast. So if you have a spare minute after finishing this episode, then we'll be grateful if you'd follow the link and answer the questions. And if you're on Spotify, you should be able to answer a quick survey question directly in the Spotify app.

That's all for now, though. We'll be back next week with more stories from the world of science. Until then, you can reach out to us on BlueSky or X, or you can send an email to podcast at nature.com. I'm Benjamin Thompson. And I'm Nick Pertuchow. Thanks for listening.

This message is brought to you by Apple Pay. If you've ever stood in the middle of a Tokyo convenience store fumbling for coins while a queue behind you grows dangerously long, you'll understand why I now use Apple Pay for basically everything. It's already on my phone, which is usually in my hand anyway, and you just tap with Apple Pay wherever you see the contactless symbol. From bento boxes to bullet trains to booking hotels on the fly, it just works.

What surprised me most, though, is how genuinely seamless it is. It makes paying feel less like a transaction and more like a tap-and-go superpower. Security is built in with Face ID, so your card number is always secured. Even if I somehow lose my phone, my Apple Pay information stays protected, which is great because I can't even keep track of my umbrella, let alone a wallet. Set up Apple Pay now and enjoy an easier way to pay. Whether you're picking up groceries or on the go, pay the Apple way.

Apple Pay is a service provided by Apple Payment Services, LLC, a subsidiary of Apple Inc. Any card used in Apple Pay is offered by the card issuer. Terms apply. If you're a lineman in charge of keeping the lights on, Grainger understands that you go to great lengths and sometimes heights to ensure the power is always flowing, which is why you can count on Grainger for professional-grade products and next-day delivery so you have everything you need to get the job done.

Call 1-800-GRANGER, click granger.com, or just stop by. Ranger, for the ones who get it done.