We're sunsetting PodQuest on 2025-07-28. Thank you for your support!
Export Podcast Subscriptions
cover of episode Who are you calling a Neanderthal?

Who are you calling a Neanderthal?

2025/5/5
logo of podcast Unexplainable

Unexplainable

AI Deep Dive Transcript
People
主持人
专注于电动车和能源领域的播客主持人和内容创作者。
Topics
主持人: 我们对尼安德特人的刻板印象,即他们愚笨、野蛮,并非完全基于科学事实,而是深受历史偏见和文化背景的影响。从Marcelin Boulle对尼安德特人骨骼的误读开始,媒体的传播和当时盛行的颅相学,都强化了这种刻板印象。然而,随着时间的推移和科学技术的进步,人们对尼安德特人的认知也在不断修正。后来的研究表明,尼安德特人并非如我们之前想象的那样低等,他们拥有复杂的社会结构和认知能力。 Paige Madison: 对尼安德特人的刻板印象并非单一原因造成,而是多种因素共同作用的结果。当时的科学环境已预设了尼安德特人愚笨的结论,Marcelin Boulle的误读只是其中一个因素。更重要的是,当时的社会和科学背景,例如盛行的颅相学,已经预设了对尼安德特人的偏见。要避免这种偏见,需要提出更广泛的问题,并重视与预期不符的证据。 Hélène Rougier: 作为一名古人类学家,我强调在研究中重视那些与预期不符的证据。这些证据可能挑战我们已有的假设,并帮助我们更客观地认识尼安德特人。例如,在对洞穴中发现的骨骼进行碳十四年代测定后,发现其年代比预期要年轻,这促使我们重新审视对该洞穴居住者的最初假设。 Paige Madison: The common narrative about Neanderthals being dumb stems from Marcelin Boulle's misinterpretation of a Neanderthal skeleton in the early 20th century. Newspapers spread his view, shaping public perception. However, this is only one piece of a larger story. The scientific community already held assumptions about Neanderthals being brutish and less intelligent. Craniometry, a now-discredited practice of measuring skulls to assess intelligence, further fueled these biases. The prevailing scientific climate of the time heavily influenced interpretations of Neanderthal remains. To avoid such biases, we must ask broader questions and seriously consider evidence that contradicts our expectations. Hélène Rougier: In my research, I emphasize the importance of considering evidence that doesn't fit our preconceived notions. Such evidence can challenge our assumptions and lead to a more objective understanding of Neanderthals. For example, carbon dating of bones found in a cave revealed a younger age than expected, forcing a reevaluation of our initial assumptions about the cave's inhabitants.

Deep Dive

Shownotes Transcript

Translations:
中文

This episode is brought to you by Tinder. You ever see someone on Tinder that takes your breath away? Now, with Tinder's first impressions, you can send a message to help stand out before you match. Try gassing up their hottest take, bond with them over a weirdly specific obsession, or hit them with a joke they have to respond to. Try Tinder's first impressions feature and explore all of the possibilities for yourself. Tinder. It starts with a swipe. Download Tinder today.

This episode is brought to you by Sweet Tarts Candy. Sweet Tarts dare to combine unexpected flavors. And when they did, something unbelievable was created. Sweet Tarts Ropes. The perfect combination of sweet and tart flavors in a soft and chewy rope. Sweet Tarts Ropes aren't just the candy you grab when you want a little treat. They're the candy you deserve. Sweet Tarts. Dare to combine. Visit SweetTartsCandy.com to shop now. Today on Unexplainable, we've got a big announcement.

In the vast audio wilderness, something peculiar is afoot. This is unexplainable. Nestled in the dense thickets of your podcast feed, a science show is growing. Unexplainable. Expanding. This is unexplainable. Listen closely, and you can even hear it multiply. Unexplainable. Suddenly, from one, there are two. More unexplainable. Unexplainable.

Twice a week. Many. Follow for new episodes every Monday and Wednesday.

All right. Hello, Bird. Hello, Noam. What are we talking about? Okay, so as you know, we recently did a whole episode on Neanderthals. Neanderthals. Neanderthals. But what you might not know is how to pronounce Neanderthals. What you might not know is that I also went on kind of a...

I guess like a side quest is the best way to describe it for that episode. So I was talking to like a bunch of different people sort of about how we'd perceived Neanderthals over time, essentially. So if I were to ask you sort of what the classic view of Neanderthals was, what would you say? Oh, um...

My brain immediately goes to the Geico commercial, I think. It's so easy to use. Geico.com. A caveman could do it. Yeah, I think that's a good example, actually, because the whole joke of those commercials is that, you know, you have so easy a caveman can do it, and then you have...

A guy who looks a lot like our classic image of a Neanderthal, right? Being like... What? What do you mean? Like, I'm super competent. Walking upright, discovering fire. But it turns out that these commercials, like, kind of have a point. Like, there are headlines about how...

Neanderthals weren't less intelligent than early modern humans or, you know, just Neanderthals weren't stupid, essentially. Were these articles written by the caveman from Geico? No, it's just a coincidence that the byline is H. Neanderthalensis. But so I was reading through these various articles about Neanderthal intelligence and I started to wonder, essentially, why do we think they're stupid? Like, where did this idea even come from to begin with?

And it turns out there's a really deep path to that. There's a very strong reason why we tend to think of Neanderthals as these kind of brutish, dumb, lesser Homo sapiens. So I reached out to Paige Madison, who's a science writer, but she also wrote her PhD thesis on our perception of Neanderthals.

And she says that there's this kind of like a common story that a lot of people point to to explain why we think Neanderthals are dumb.

And it starts with this, like, anthropologist slash paleontologist slash geologist guy named Marcelin Boulle. And so as the story goes, this French scientist, Marcelin Boulle, misinterpreted a Neanderthal skeleton. So this was the early 20th century, and people had been kind of digging up bits and pieces of Neanderthals for a while, right?

But this time, some diggers had found a more complete skeleton in France. And they sent this Neanderthal skeleton to Boulle, who was at the Natural History Museum in Paris. He got a hold of one of the first really complete specimens, and he took a look at it and decided that these were these hunched-over brutes that were so dumb that they couldn't even really stand up straight. And again, as the story goes...

this is what people ran with. Like, newspapers spread Boo's version of Neanderthals, and that's how it became sort of the dominant perception. And then, decades later, in the late 1950s, people reexamined the skeletons and were essentially like, so...

This is the skeleton of an old man with arthritis. Okay. So, like, this is kind of the equivalent of basically, like, if someone were trying to figure out, like, what were people like in the 21st century? And they only looked at, like, the skeleton of one old man. Or, like, if someone found, like, Shaq's skeleton, you'd be like, oh, yeah, humans. They're all, like, seven feet tall. Exactly.

So this Bull story is compelling, right? And it is true that Bull did publish this paper in the early 1900s, that his vision of the arthritic Neanderthal was republished by newspapers, right? It did contribute to the narrative that Neanderthals were stupid. But Page says that his image of the sort of brutish Neanderthal was...

is actually just one part of a much larger story here. Fool didn't create this image alone. It was more of a confluence of factors that happened all around the same time. She says instead of looking at sort of one scientist, it's actually more interesting to kind of look at the scientific waters that he was swimming in. And her contention is that those scientific waters...

We're actually already assuming, essentially, that Neanderthals were brutish and boorish and stupid at the time. Interesting. So, essentially, like, when Europeans first got interested in Neanderthal skulls, like the middle of the 1800s, people were also getting into sort of craniometry. You know what craniometry is, right? That's like the...

The skull measuring thing that I guess the Nazis, I tend to associate with the Nazis, which I guess is later. But the idea is they would measure skull sizes of various ethnic groups and they would say that, oh, we can determine your ethnic group based on the shape and size of your skull. And that correlates with how smart you are or how good of a person you are or how much you deserve to live. Right. Right.

And I guess to be sort of abundantly clear here, right, like we do now know that skull measuring is not a useful way of understanding people's mental capacity or their other traits, right? But if we go back to the 19th century, this logic was really popular, right? Like a lot of scientists had bought into this. And so they would look at, for example,

And so the idea was that like if you had a prominent brow ridge, you were somehow more primitive. And then if you had like a

a steep forehead, say, they thought your brain was more developed. And so scientists would argue that, like, Europeans were superior because their foreheads were steeper. And again, these are features that we know, first of all, those differences are minuscule, and they are certainly not meaningful in terms of intellect and cognition. But at the time, they were seen as incredibly meaningful and a way that you could differentiate these different groups.

And Neanderthal skulls, as they were digging them up, they kind of fit perfectly into this narrative. They fit in exactly the spot that these European scientists were categorizing as the lower end of human intellect and sort of the more primitive end. So basically, like, when the scientist, Boo, like, looks at this Neanderthal skeleton, he's kind of seeing, like, what he expects to see in the context of, like,

The science leading up to this is essentially saying like, "Hey, Neanderthals seem to be kind of stupid." And he's like, "Yeah, and they hunched too." Yeah. I think what he did was he took it one step further. So he kind of applied this brutish conception that had already existed and applied it to their posture. And so, of course, yes, that is significant. It did partially shape how we think about them. But he certainly did not invent it by any means.

And it's worth mentioning, too, that the signs of arthritis on the skeleton are well recognized. And Boole probably should have been able to recognize them. I mean, there's no reason, given his training, that he wouldn't. So it kind of goes to show how our expectations can lead us towards a certain conclusion and kind of like push us in that direction, even when...

you know, the evidence isn't quite there. And that's why you see these interpretations change over time, right? It's because there's so much else that's going into the interpretation. It's not as simple as looking at the bones and immediately knowing exactly what they meant, but that is being filtered through, you know, tons of other information, both scientific and cultural. You know, we just can't turn off that lens at any given moment.

So this is like, this is just saying it's not this one scientist's fault. It's sort of like, why was everyone else around so ready to believe this? It's because everyone kind of was in the same context. Right. And this idea, it's reinforced if you look at the re-examination of the skeleton that happened later on. So different historians will suggest different times for when people sort of started changing their perspective on Neanderthals and Neanderthal intelligence. But for

For Paige, she starts to see people rethinking things after World War II. And again, like the Nazis were involved in this project, right, of skull measuring and using those measurements to justify horrific things, horrific race categorization.

And so it's not that surprising to Page as a historian that as you start to have people after the war re-evaluating skull measurement science, that's also when you start to see people maybe changing their perspectives or starting to change their perspectives on Neanderthals as well. So you just had all of these factors kind of lining up that suddenly the earlier ideas about Neanderthals just didn't make as much sense.

That's fascinating. I guess the place that leaves me is just, I guess I want to know, how do you change the social context? I assume you can't. And then how do you know when you're in a social context? Are you asking basically like, what do we do about current times? How do we know if what we think of Neanderthals right now is right or anything is right if we're not sure about the water we're swimming in? Yeah, I mean, great question. Stay tuned.

You know that feeling when someone shows up for you just when you need it most? That's what Uber is all about. Not just a ride or dinner at your door. It's how Uber helps you show up for the moments that matter. Because showing up can turn a tough day around or make a good one even better. Whatever it is, big or small, Uber is on the way. So you can be on yours. Uber, on our way. This episode is brought to you by Indeed.

When your computer breaks, you don't wait for it to magically start working again. You fix the problem. So why wait to hire the people your company desperately needs? Use Indeed's sponsored jobs to hire top talent fast. And even better, you only pay for results. There's no need to wait. Speed up your hiring with a $75 sponsored job credit at indeed.com slash podcast. Terms and conditions apply.

So before the break, you basically asked like,

how do we know we're not being misled by our own cultural context, right? And when I asked Paige this question, she was essentially like, yeah, we live in a society. You know, like, inevitably, we're going to have some degree of our cultural moment acting as the lens that we see through. And she even said that, you know, some scholars say that our ideas about Neanderthals have been

often said more about us than they have about Neanderthals. It's a little bit like holding a mirror up to ourselves. But she also says that there are ways to try and kind of avoid this trap. Okay. So one way that she describes is just...

keeping your questions really broad. So for example, if you find artwork in a cave and you assume that artwork is something that only Homo sapiens have done and that Neanderthals were not capable of it, then you never even ask that question. You just ask which Homo sapiens did this and when. But if the worldview has changed and you come into a cave and you see that there's art in there, you can then ask,

who did this in a more open way. And that's something I work with with scientists a lot, is just thinking about the ways that just their starting points, their questions, have already either opened or closed certain possibilities. And then I also talked to a paleoanthropologist named Hélène Rougier. So she is actively studying early modern humans and Neanderthals. And I asked her, basically, like,

How do you avoid having your cultural assumptions color your science, right? And she said that one of the main things that scientists can do is to look for evidence that doesn't fit what they'll assume they'll find and kind of zero in on that. Like specifically look for things that contradict your expectations. Or like if you see them, don't dismiss them, right? So she gave me this really...

basic example from her work kind of where she was she was looking through a cave and she found some bones and she sent them out to be to be carbon dated and when they came back they were they were younger than she expected them to be and she said that she she could have just dismissed it right she could have said that's not possible there must have been contamination at the lab or something like forget about these bones and

But instead, she kind of zeroed in on these surprising dates and ended up realizing that her initial assumptions about who had lived in this cave and when they'd lived there just hadn't been right. And so, like, this is a really small example, but it's part of that overall thesis she has that, like, one of the few ways that you can kind of check your biases is to look for pieces of evidence that don't

fit with your biases and then take those pieces of evidence seriously. Interesting. I mean, that does feel, I got to say, it feels like easier said than done. I mean, if I were going to make a Geico commercial for science, it would be, science, so difficult science.

Perhaps even a Neanderthal, despite all of its, you know, potential sophistication and intelligence, might have difficulty doing it. Oh my God. And that's why I don't work in advertising. Paige Madison is a science writer who wrote a journal article on this topic, and she's writing an upcoming book on human origins.

This episode was produced by me, Bird Pinkerton. It was edited by Meredith Hodnot with help from Jorge Just. Mara also runs the show. Noam Hassenfeld writes the music. Christian Ayala does our mixing and our sound design. Melissa Hirsch checked the facts. Julia Longoria is the fact that glass frogs can have transparent skin. And as always, we are grateful to Brian Resnick for co-creating the show. This was our first ever Monday episode. Do you have thoughts about how it went?

Do you have ideas for future Monday episodes? Write in to unexplainable at Vox.com. If you want to support our show and help us keep making it, please join our membership program. That's at Vox.com slash members. Vox.com slash members. You can also support us by leaving us a nice review or a rating or just by telling people in your life to listen.

Unexplainable is part of the Vox Media Podcast Network, and we will be back on Wednesday.